Now that I have your attention I will do my best to keep it – but
you cannot expect me to attempt to answer such an important question in just a few
paragraphs.
It might be appropriate to begin by issuing a health warning
about my ability to answer such a complex question. I can claim a great deal of
interest in the subject, but my expertise is limited.
In my view human happiness is ultimately about having a
meaningful life – one that is meaningful to the person living it - but that is certainly
not a secret. Wise people have been saying similar things for thousands of
years.
What I am about to write about now has to do with momentary
happiness and the way we pursue our goals. It seems like a good idea for
individuals to pursue their goals in ways that enable them to experience many
happy moments and not too much disappointment along the way.
Some recent research on the links between risk-taking,
expectations, rewards and happiness has produced some interesting findings. I
propose to present some of those findings in a somewhat novel way and to
combine them with some additional speculations.
The research by Robb Rutledge (of University College,
London) and colleagues involved presenting participants in a decision-making game
with choices between certain and risky options and repeatedly asking them to
report their momentary happiness. The study used fMRI to examine the
relationship between happiness reports and neural responses. The study also
made use of the Great Brain Experiment app, to test results on large numbers of
people playing the decision-making game on smart phones and tablets. (The game
is actually still available the Great Brain Experiment site and good fun to
play.) The research is reported in an article entitled ‘A Computational and
neural model of momentary subjective well-being’, recently published in PNAS.
Good summaries of the article have been published by ‘The Telegraph’ and ‘The Atlantic’.
The study found that momentary happiness is determined by
the combined influence of recent reward expectations and prediction errors
arising from those expectations. The happiness equation takes the following
form:
Happiness = baseline average mood + what you can settle for (CR) + what
you'll get on average if you gamble (EV) + the difference between that and what
you actually get (RPE).
The equation takes account of the fact that memory
fades, so that more recent events have a larger impact on happiness than
earlier events.
The findings suggest that happiness depends not on how well things are
going, but on whether they are going better than expected.
It is a mistake to interpret the findings as in the headline in one paper: “The
secret of happiness? LOWER your expectations: A good day is when things are
going better than expected”. One reason the results don’t imply that people
should lower their expectations is because the measure of expectation used is
average reward, rather than a direct measure of optimism or pessimism. It would
be slightly more accurate to interpret the findings as suggesting that people
can avoid disappointment by staying within their comfort zone instead of
choosing riskier options that involve high average (expected) reward. But that
interpretation is also inadequate because it overlooks the pleasure that people
get from anticipating a high reward (even if the outcome is not as good as expected)
and also overlooks the buzz that some people get when they gamble (choose an
option with high potential reward but lower average reward) and win.
I have attempted to map out some relationships between expectations,
forecast errors and happiness in the following two charts.
In the first chart, we begin in Quadrant A, with expectations and
happiness as indicated by the blue line i.e. a happiness rating above 7.
Unfortunately, it turns out that outcomes are below expectations, so in
Quadrant B it is apparent that we have a negative prediction error of -3. When
we translate that to the X axis in Quadrant C (using the 450 line in
the south-east quadrant) we find that an error of -3 corresponds to a happiness
rating of less than 6.
So, the question now arises of how you should respond to that
disappointment. One way to respond is to get back into your comfort zone and
adopt a strategy involving lower expectations and no prediction error. That
strategy avoids disappointment but it means that you forgo the pleasure of
contemplating the happiness that you could expect, on average, under the original
strategy.
An alternative response is depicted in the
second chart. That involves sticking with the original strategy but improving
your luck.
An improvement in luck is shown by a shift in the
relationship in Quadrant B. By improving your luck you are able to achieve an
outcome better than expected and end up happier than you expected.
So, the secret of happiness is to get lucky!
Jokes aside, it
makes sense to stick to a strategy that you have good reason to think will
yield high returns over the longer term, even if you experience disappointing
results in the short term. In other words, the secret of happiness is adopt the
strategy that you expect to yield greatest rewards over the longer term and “stay
the course”.
Some readers might question the wisdom of that on the
grounds that most people tend to be optimistic in their expectations, relative
to average reward (or mathematical expectation). However, the findings of a recent paper by Gigi Foster and Paul Frijters, which examines the formation of
expectations by undergraduates at two Australian universities, suggests that
optimistic expectations are benign. The results suggest that apart from their
direct contribution to happiness, optimistic expectations motivate people to
work harder to achieve their goals.
So, adding all that together, the secret of happiness would
be to adopt the strategy that you expect to yield greatest rewards over the
longer term, and to back your expectations by staying the course and working
hard. But you already knew that! And something
important seems to be missing.
The real secret of happiness, in my view, is to play the inner frame games
of self-acceptance and cheerfulness, and to adopt an attitude of awe and
fascination about the world.
Postscript:
I linked to the wrong article by Gigi Foster and Paul Frijters. An abstract of the article I meant to to link to can be found here.
Postscript:
I linked to the wrong article by Gigi Foster and Paul Frijters. An abstract of the article I meant to to link to can be found here.
We are bombarded with advertising that usually equates happiness with having the right car or clothes. In recent years our purchasing power has increased, which means we can afford to buy more things and yet buying more stuff has not made us any happier, because happiness is not for sale
ReplyDeleteilchi lee best selling author